Penny Ur shares research she finds interesting at IATEFL 2023
Penny Ur posting for photos with fans after her IATEFL talk (19.4.23) |
This is the second instalment of my two-part post about Penny Ur’s session at IATEFL matter-of-factly entitled Interesting recent research, in which, in a span of 30 minutes, I learned more than during the rest of the conference.
Click HERE for the first part.
The reason why it’s taken me so long to publish this (IATEFL took place in April) is that it’s not merely a summary of the talk. Upon my return I did some digging into the sources Penny Ur cited; the result is a summary combined with some personal reflections. By her own admission, she whizzed through so many studies in the talk that I only focus on those that are interesting to me.
The previous part covered the age factor, the use of L1 and error correction; in this one we turn to the following three strands of research: inferencing meaning from context, the flipped classroom and the use of pictures.
Inferencing meaning from context
Another Penny Ur’s hobbyhorse. She laid the groundwork by referencing some early studies, such as Laufer & Bensoussan (1984), in which students guessed correctly less than a quarter of target items in a text. Making a case for explicit vocabulary teaching, Laufer and Bensoussan argued that mere exposure to new vocabulary would not lead to vocabulary learning.
Similarly, in Nassaji's (2003) study, learners could accurately guess less than 50% of the target words, according to Ur (in fact, the number of correct guesses was very similar to that in Laufer & Bensoussan, at 25%). “And he was playing fair with them”, she remarked – 95% of words in the text were familiar to learners (knowing at least 95% of words in a text, or 95% lexical coverage, is commonly considered the threshold for adequate reading comprehension – see, for example, Laufer & Ravenhorst-Kalovski 2010). Nassaji concurs that guessing is not an effective way of enriching vocabulary because of limitations inherent in inferring meaning from contextual clues.
More recent experimental studies compared guessing from context with other vocabulary learning strategies showing that the latter are more effective. In Zou’s experiment (2018), dictionary consultation led to better learning of word meanings while in Ko’s (2012) study providing glosses for unknown words either in L1 (Korean) or L2 (English) resulted in significantly better learning than guessing from context, which the author refers to as “risky and misleading”. Interestingly, the participants expressed a preference for L2 glosses, especially if they are concise and clear.
A recent review (Xiali & Altunel 2018) of empirical research on lexical inferencing concludes that depending solely on contextual clues is not an effective vocabulary learning strategy, particularly for lower L2 proficiency levels. The paper ends, however, with a recommendation to use concordance lines for vocabulary learning. The strategy does rely on contextual clues, but since learners are presented with several examples of the target word in context, they have a greater opportunity to figure out its meaning.
Further thoughts and practical implications
Studying concordance lines not only helps learners with the meaning of the target word, but also, perhaps more importantly, with how the word is used. It provides so called ‘condensed exposure’ or ‘condensed reading’ (Gabrielatos 2005) and is one application of Data-Driven Learning (DDL), which I delve into in my recent chapter in Demystifying Corpus Linguistics for English Language Teachers (Harrington & Ronan 2023). When it comes to resources available online, Nik Peachey has some good ideas HERE.
In general, it is important to distinguish between guessing from context as a vocabulary learning strategy and an exam strategy. Yes, it may not be effective for the former – and personally I have never relied on it as a source of vocabulary development – but it may come in handy when learners grapple with unknown words in a reading passage on an exam. See some useful suggestions on the EAP Foundation website: www.eapfoundation.com/reading/skills/guess/
The flipped classroom
Just to remind you, in the flipped classroom model students engage with instructional materials before class (e.g. a recorded lecture), thereby freeing up class time for discussions, pairwork and other interactive activities as well as practice activities which traditionally might be given for homework.
Further thoughts and practical implications
Pictures
Further thoughts and practical implications
My attempt at supporting the learning of the verb refuse with an image |
References
Only additional references not mentioned in the talk are listed here:Laufer, B., & Ravenhorst-Kalovski, G. C. (2010). Lexical threshold revisited: Lexical text coverage, learners’ vocabulary size and reading comprehension. Reading in a Foreign Language, 22(1), 15–30. Available from http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/rfl/
Gabrielatos, C. (2005). Corpora and language teaching: Just a fling or wedding bells? TESL-EJ 8(4): 1-35. Available from http://tesl-ej.org/ej32/a1.htm
I appreciate your detailed breakdown of the research strands she covered, along with your personal reflections on each topic. Could you elaborate on one specific research finding or idea that resonated with you the most? Tel U
ReplyDeleteThanks for the summary.
ReplyDelete